AAC Modeling and Language Acquisition

an article review


IMG_6649.jpg

The early stages of learning any new language is immersion into that language, seeing others use it, and interacting with others who are using it. The same is true for AAC users with Complex Communication Needs.

Being immersed in the language of AAC through modeling (aka, language input), where multiple communication partners are using their AAC system when speaking to them, can significantly improve language development in a number of ways. According to Sennott, Light, and McNaughton (2016), clients can greatly benefit from AAC modeling by their communication partners in the following aspects of language:


1. Pragmatic Language

unsplash-image-9x3jGcu3qQ0.jpg

Eight beginning communicators aged 4 to 8 years old, showed an increase in the frequency of communicative turns using AAC on SGDs, in the context of shared story book reading.

2. Semantic Knowledge

unsplash-image-oHoBIbDj7lo.jpg

Seventy-two participants aged 2 to 12 years, revealed an increase in vocabulary knowledge in response to AAC modeling during story book reading/arts and crafts/meal times, as well as modest speech increases, reinforcing the notion that AAC intervention does not impede speech development.


3. Syntax

unsplash-image-CiUs9eCvNb8.jpg

Eleven participants, aged 2 to 5 years, demonstrated an increase in multi-symbol utterances in the context of play and shared storybook reading.



4. Morphology

unsplash-image-7PYqjNzvrc4.jpg

Three participants, aged 6 to 11 years, showed improvements in the acquisition of morphemes such as “plural -S,” “present progressive -ING,” “past tense -ED,” and “possessive ’S,’” in the context of book reading.






Summary

Ultimately, 10 studies were included in this evidence-based analysis: nine case studies and one group design. The studies included a total of 94 children, aged two to twelve years, with a range of disabilities: nine children with cerebral palsy, seven children with Down syndrome, and 15 children with other disabilities (autism, developmental delay, childhood apraxia of speech, cystic hygroma, velopharyngeal insufficiency, DiGeorge syndrome, and Prader–Willi syndrome).

Communication partners across the 10 studies included speech therapists, parents, and educational or clinical assistants.

Five of the studies are considered ‘partner-training interventions’ where the researchers trained parents and educational supporters and then measured the partner–child dyad’s performance during treatment.

Future research is needed to determine the roles of language level, intellectual functioning, and fast mapping ability, as well as whether AAC modeling is an effective intervention across the life span.














References

Sennott, S. C., Light, J. C., McNaughton, D. (2016). AAC Modeling Intervention Research Review. Research and Practice for Persons with Severe Disabilities, 41(2), 101-115. doi:10.1177/1540796916638822d

IMG_6650.jpg
Heather IsmayComment